Bookshelf

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Cover of Measurement of Human Stress: A Multidimensional Approach

Measurement of Human Stress: A Multidimensional Approach

Achsah Dorsey , Elissa Scherer , Randy Eckhoff , and Robert Furberg .

Research Triangle Park (NC): RTI Press ; 2022 Jun . © 2022 RTI International.

This work is distributed under the terms of a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 license (CC BY-NC-ND), a copy of which is available at https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0

Stress is a multidimensional construct that comprises exposure to events, perceptions of stress, and physiological responses to stress. Research consistently demonstrates a strong association between stress and a myriad of physical and mental health concerns, resulting in a pervasive and interdisciplinary agreement on the importance of investigating the relationship between stress and health. Developing a holistic understanding of stress requires assessment of the three domains vital to the study of stress: (1) the presence of environmental stressors, (2) psychological and biological reactions to stressors, and (3) the length of time over which the stressor or stress response occurs. Research into all three domains requires multiple methods. Self-reports allow for subjective evaluations of stress that illuminate the duration and severity of the psychological response to stressors. Biomarkers, in turn, capture a more-objective measure of stress and create a deeper understanding of the biological response to chronic and acute stress. Finally, the use of digital biomarkers allows for further exploration of the physiological fluctuations caused by stress by measuring the changes occurring at the same time as the stressor. Future research on stress and health should favor a multidimensional approach that creates a triangulated picture of stress, drawing from each of the three aforementioned method groups.

Introduction

Stress is consistently associated with a myriad of physical and mental morbidities, as well as mortality. 1 Additionally, the financial burden of stress is estimated to be $300 billion per year in the United States alone. 2 As a result, there is pervasive and interdisciplinary agreement on the importance of understanding and alleviating stress to improve quality of life. However, the measurement of stress varies greatly within the large body of work linking stress and well-being. Researchers attempting to quantify stress should first understand the measurement strategies available to them and the strengths and limitations of each form of measurement. The goal of this paper is to define stress and offer an overview of measurement methods available to researchers interested in capturing stress.

Box

Key Research Needs.

Defining Stress

The definition of stress has evolved over several decades, and a universally adopted definition has yet to be reached. Seminal models of stress proposed by Selye in the 1950s define stress as the “non-specific response of the body to any demand for change” or the “rate of wear and tear on the body.” 3 Proposed refinements to these definitions in the decades since have differentiated between stressors, defined as stimuli that challenge an organism’s biological and psychological equilibrium (i.e., homeostasis), and the stress response, defined as the process through which the organism attempts to restore homeostasis. 4 Although the revised definitions differentiate between the cause and effect of stress, they have frequently received critique for being too vague, as nearly all activities that an organism undertakes constitute a challenge to homeostasis. 5 One model created to further clarify a working definition of stress, the psychophysiological stress concept, has been widely adopted in stress-related research since its dissemination in 2011. 5 Developed by Koolhaas and colleagues, the psychophysiological model of stress defines stress as the perceived or anticipated inability to successfully cope with situations that are not predictable or controllable. 6 This inability to cope with an experience leads to both subjective feelings of stress and objective biological changes in the body. 6

In defining stress, it is also important to recognize that stress can be conceptualized both as an acute reaction and a chronically accumulated state throughout the life course. Taken with the information above, this highlights three domains vital to the study of stress: (1) the presence of environmental stressors, (2) psychological and biological reactions to stressors, and (3) the length of time over which the stressor or stress response occurs. 7 Many subjective and objective measures have been developed and validated to quantify these domains. The following sections will cover not only subjective measures that capture perception of environmental stressors and psychological response but also objective measures that capture biological change.

Measures of Self-Report

Self-report measures of stress consist of series of questions or prompts that inquire about respondents’ lived experience with various components of stress. Several measures have been developed to quantify both reactions to acute stressors and the accumulation of chronic stress (Table 1).

Table 1

Inventory of subjective (self-report) measures of acute and chronic stress.

Common measures to assess chronic stress include both the Life Events and Difficulties Schedule (LEDS) 20 and the Trier Inventory for Chronic Stress (TICS). 17 , 21 The LEDS captures exposure to severe acute events (i.e., those lasting less than 1 month) and severe chronic difficulties (i.e., those lasting longer than 1 month) over the previous year through a semi-structured interview that prompts the individual to recall 95 possible life events. The individual provides additional context around each experience; a trained expert then codes the context. LEDS stressors are grouped into 10 domains: education, work, reproduction, housing, money/possessions, crime/legal, health/treatment/accidents, marital/partner relationship, other relationships, and miscellaneous. The TICS attempts to capture chronic stress through a more-structured questionnaire covering nine factors of chronic stress: “work overload, social overload, pressure to perform, work discontent, excessive demands at work, lack of social recognition, social tensions, social isolation, and chronic worrying. Participants in these surveys rate 57 items covering how often situations within the nine domains occur (i.e., never, rarely, sometimes, often, or very often) over a recall period of 3 months.

Several measures have been designed to capture stress over a shorter period of time, including the Cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) and the Stress Overload Scale (SOS), among others (Table 1). The most commonly used measure for assessing global stress perceptions is the 10-item PSS. 15 This survey captures the degree to which a person views their life as uncontrollable, unpredictable, and overloaded in the past month. Scores are calculated using a five-point scale (0 = never, 1 = almost never, 2 = once in a while, 3 = often, and 4 = very often) that is summed for a total score, where higher scores represent a greater level of perceived stress. The PSS has been translated into a variety of languages, 22 , 23 which allows for its use in both English and non-English-speaking populations. As an alternative to the PSS, the SOS was designed to measure when stress overwhelms a person’s coping mechanisms. 13 Twenty-four statements touch on personal vulnerability and event load, and an additional six items are used to discourage inconsistent responses. A shorter version of the SOS (10 items) also exists. 24 Each of the statements is evaluated on a 5-point graded-response scale (1 = not at all, 5 = a lot) to investigate feelings and experiences from the prior week.

Though measures such as the PSS or the SOS capture stress over a shorter time than measures of more-chronic stress, they are still frequently criticized for having great potential for bias because individuals are asked to report retrospectively on cumulative stress experienced. In response to this limitation, the ecological momentary assessment (EMA) method was developed. EMA is the repeated measurement of an individual’s subjective experience of stress in the context of daily life. 25 This method can be used to capture momentary stress at multiple points throughout the day, which can then be aggregated to create a picture of stress levels over time. As technology has evolved, the smartphone has become a popular tool for conducting EMAs and contextualized survey items, including single-item measures. 26 For example, a question prompted by a beep on a responder’s phone, such as “How stressed were you right before the beep went off?” can capture real-time stress with a variety of response options (not at all, a little, moderately, quite a bit, and extremely). 27

One measure used for EMAs is the Photographic Affect Meter (PAM). PAM is a validated, one-item, mobile application–based measure of affect that correlates with the widely used Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). 8 As shown in Figure 1, subjects select an image from a grid of 16 pictures that best represents their current emotional state.

Figure 1

The Photographic Affect Meter (PAM).

PAM is used to construct a set of numbers for the purpose of quantitative analyses. The first is a categorical 1–16 score that maps on to the PANAS scale. 9 Consistent with the conceptualization of the PANAS, this affect scale ranges from low valence (the level of pleasure that an event generates) and arousal (the level of autonomic activation that an event creates) to high pleasure and arousal. Therefore, PAM also produces two separate affect scores for valence and arousal, which are calculated according to the chosen image’s position in the 16-image grid. Valence is computed using a 1 to 4 score (negative, slightly negative, slightly positive, positive) based on the column position of the image. Arousal is computed in a similar fashion using the row position of the image, with 1 representing calm or low arousal and 4 representing excitement or high arousal.

Self-report measures are useful tools in establishing subjective evaluations of stress levels. They are often cost-effective and correlated with clinical endpoints of interest, but they can be lengthy to administer and are subject to several types of bias. 28 – 30 Self-report measures are best used in combination with objective biological or digital markers of stress to understand the comprehensive impact on outcomes of interest. 6

Biomarkers

Stressors cause a variety of biological responses. These physiological reactions can be measured using biomarkers that represent the stress response from the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, the autonomic nervous system (ANS), and the immune system (Table 2). 48

Table 2

Inventory of objective (chemical and digital) measures of acute and chronic stress.

The HPA axis is particularly responsive to psychosocial stress. One output of this system is cortisol, a hormone secreted from the adrenal gland that plays a vital role in cognition, metabolism, and immune function as well as the stress response. Levels of cortisol in the body fluctuate daily in response to routine events, unexpected stressors, and circadian patterns that render salivary cortisol highest in the morning and lowest at night. 49 , 50 In addition to daily variation, cortisol may be chronically high or low. Both the acute fluctuation and the chronic compounding of cortisol are of interest to researchers. Acute cortisol change can be reliably measured in saliva and blood, cortisol in urine can be used to establish daily cortisol secretion, 42 and cortisol in hair can be used to gather long-term cortisol levels over weeks or months. 51 Salivary and hair cortisol are commonly used in studies investigating stress because collection is relatively quick and easy. Assessment of cortisol in saliva captures acute cortisol production over the past 15–20 minutes and is often used to measure daily stress.

Collecting saliva samples is comparatively easy and painless (a cotton swab is placed in the mouth of a participant and then sealed in a container), but a single saliva cortisol sample tells little about levels of stress over an extended period of time, and collection time is incredibly important, as cortisol levels vary across the day. Therefore, multiple samples of salivary cortisol throughout the day are needed to accurately capture daily stress. Cortisol levels measured in hair are used as a longer-term biomarker of HPA axis activity because cortisol is incorporated into the hair as it grows. 52 Thus, hair cortisol captures a much longer period: a 1-cm hair segment reflects total cortisol secretion in the past month. These cumulative levels of hair cortisol do not provide information about HPA axis activity specific to any particular time within the measurement period.

The ANS modulates organ system activity through autonomic reflexes that respond to internal and external stimuli. Catecholamines, including norepinephrine and epinephrine, are hormones made by the adrenal glands that serve as strong biomarkers of ANS activity, specifically sympathetic-adrenal medullary (SAM) activity. 53 .SAM activity occurs as a rapid response to a stressor to produce alertness and aid in responsive decision making. By contrast, HPA axis function is more representative of exposure to repeated stressors. Catecholamines can be captured acutely through blood concentration and as a short-term biomarker of daily stress through a 24-hour urine collection protocol. 42 , 43 Alpha-amylase, an enzyme that breaks down starch, is another biomarker of ANS activity and can be detected in saliva samples. 40 Like cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine found in the urine, alpha-amylase fluctuates over the day, so multiple measures are needed to properly understand changes in alpha-amylase in response to stressors. 40

Another way to capture stress response is to measure immune activation. 54 Cytokines are a group of proteins that serve as signaling molecules that help mediate and regulate immune function and can be measured from blood samples. 54 Interleukin (IL)-6 is one type of cytokine that plays a vital role in the development of fever and regulates the acute inflammatory response in the liver. 38 Although IL-6 is correlated with the development of cardiovascular disease and other chronic inflammatory disorders, it is very sensitive and easily elevated by exercise. When collecting samples, participants should refrain from exercise for approximately 12 hours to get an accurate reading.

C-reactive protein (CRP) is another substance integral to immune system response that is used to investigate stress. 44 CRP is made in the liver and released in reaction to inflammation, and it has been linked to stress response in humans. One advantage to using CRP as a measure of stress is that it lacks diurnal variation. However, CRP values can never be diagnostic on their own, and information about other pathological markers is vital to interpreting CRP levels.

Other immune measures of stress include the measurement of antibodies for common herpes viruses, including Epstein Barr virus antibodies, herpes simplex virus type 1 (HSV-1) antibodies, and cytomegalovirus antibodies. 45 , 55 , 56 These viruses are persistent infections that require adequate cell-mediated immunity to maintain in their latent states. 57 , 58 Longer-term stress can decrease the immune system’s ability to keep these viruses from reactivating. 57 , 58 As they reactivate, increased antibodies produced in response to circulating viral particles can be used as a measure of stress. 57 , 58

The biomarkers listed here provide quantifiable measures of the various physiological pathways that lead to a biological stress response. The use of these biomarkers in stress research allows for a more-objective measure of stress and a deeper understanding of the alterations in biological systems caused by chronic and acute stress. However, researchers often assume the validity of biomarkers despite the need for continued evaluation. 59 Many stress biomarkers indicate levels of inflammation, but many factors influence inflammation, and they should be considered in the analysis. 60

The methods for obtaining biological samples for analysis may be a limitation for many researchers. Both the investigator collecting the samples and the participant need appropriate training because of the need for valid and consistent collection procedures. 61 Correct processing after sample collection, shipping requirements, and the proper storage of the biological samples are also critical for reliable results but can prove difficult in resource-limited areas.

Digital Measurement of Biomarkers

The use of sensors, software tools, and signal processing methods to explore complex biological signals related to stress has increased with recent advances in computational methods and concurrent, widespread adoption of consumer electronics. 62

Although a broad range of systems influence cardiac performance and functions, the ANS is the most prominent. 63 Like some of the aforementioned biomarkers, pulse and heart rate variability (HRV) can be used as proxies for fluctuations in the ANS in response to external and internal stressors. Pulse represents the arterial palpitation of the heartbeat and is reported in beats per minute. When individuals are exposed to stressors, their breathing and pulse can temporarily change. The alteration in heartbeat is measured through touch at any place where an artery can be compressed; two of the common areas for measuring pulse are the wrist and neck. HRV, or the fluctuating time between heart beats, can also be used as a proxy for ANS response to regulatory impulses and stress. 31 Typically, when the ANS is stimulated, variation between heart beats is low, and during relaxed states, variability is high. Advantageously, fluctuations in HRV can be measured while other physiological markers of stress remain constant. 64

HRV, as well as pulse, can be captured through different techniques with varying validity and convenience. Electrical impulses cause cardiac muscle to squeeze and pump blood from the heart, which can be captured through electrical leads attached to an electrocardiogram (ECG). The ECG method is the gold standard for measuring electrical activity of the heart. However, it is difficult to reliably collect ECG data in a cost-efficient, ecologically valid manner. In response to these challenges, wearable device manufacturers have turned to photoplethysmography (PPG) sensors. PPG is an optical measurement of blood volume variations as capillaries expand and contract with each heartbeat. 65 Data collected using wearable PPG sensors has shown varied validity when compared with the gold-standard ECG. 66 This is mostly because of differences in what PPG and ECG measure. PPG measures variability in peripheral pulse, whereas ECG directly measures the electrical cycles of heart function. Several factors, including blood pressure and age-related changes to the vascular system, influence peripheral pulse. 67 Differing device quality and sensor placement can also explain differences between ECG and PPG. Placement is most commonly on the wrist, but can also be on the forehead, earlobes, upper arm, torso, fingertips, and ankles. Each of these locations experiences unique amounts of movement, which can limit the sensor’s ability to capture an accurate PPG reading. To counter the effect of movement on PPG sensors, wearable manufacturers are incorporating three-axis accelerometer data into their heart rate algorithms. 65 As ECG and PPG wearable technology improves, large-scale health studies are producing robust, evidence-based results using user-owned devices. 68

Electrodermal activity (EDA) is another physiological measure of stress response that can be captured through sensor-based measurement. 69 , 70 EDA is the variation in electrical characteristics of the skin. These fluctuations in conductivity are caused by the changes in the ANS and are considered the most-useful index of ANS arousal. 64 EDA is measured through continuous monitoring of the involuntary changes in skin conductivity. EDA is particularly useful when assessing changing impacts of stress because of the continuous alterations in skin conductance 71 and because it is the only ANS variable associated with psychosocial stress that is not correlated with other parasympathetic markers. 64 Researchers can capture EDA through wearable devices such as the Empatica. 69

Like biomarkers, sensor-based measures provide an objective method for quantifying the physiological response to psychosocial stress. Gathering complex, multisensory data on pulse, HRV, and EDA is easy and minimally invasive because of advances in technology. The development of small, reliable, and cost-effective biometric devices has created an increase in access and interest among consumers. However, these digital measures can only capture the stress response contemporaneously. In addition, the accuracy of some commercially available biometric devices is questionable, and assessment of the data quality is needed when using this technology.

Relationships Between Subjective and Objective Measurement of Stress

Subjective and objective measures of stress are related, but not identical, constructs. The psychological experience of stress may not always translate to measurable biological change and vice versa. 5 Limited research exploring associations between subjective and objective measures exists, and that which does suggests notable, but weak, associations between the two types of measures. For example, a recent study by Weckesser and colleagues found that only 16% of the variance in hair cortisol was explained by the subjective Weekly Hassle Scale, and that hair cortisol was not significantly related to the PSS or the TICS. 6 In contrast, other studies have found statistically significant relationships between cortisol and the PSS, 72 and other self-report measures such as the SOS 13 , 73 or aggregated subjective stress measures built from a combination of other measures. 74 The relationship between perceived and objective measures of stress may be further explained by other factors, such as resilience. For example, a recent study by Lehrer and colleagues found that psychological resilience moderates the association between perceived stress (measured by the PSS) and hair cortisol; higher resilience reduces the association between perceived stress and hair cortisol. 75 Further exploration of the relationships between self-report and objective measures of stress is needed to provide greater understanding of the impacts of stress on health.

Future Research Needs

Stress is a multidimensional construct that comprises exposure to events, perceptions of stress, and physiological responses to stress. A nuanced understanding of the links between stress and health requires assessment of each of these components in both acute and chronic scenarios. Employing self-reports allows for subjective evaluations of stress that illuminate the duration and severity of the psychological response to stressors. This information is vital to understanding the physiological stress response measured by biomarkers. Biomarkers, in turn, capture a more-objective measure of stress and create a deeper understanding of the biological response to chronic and acute stress. Finally, the use of digital biomarkers allows for further exploration of the physiological fluctuations caused by stress by measuring the changes occurring at the same time as the stressor. Future research should therefore favor a multidimensional approach that creates a triangulated picture of stress, drawing from perceived measures of stress as well as chemical and digital biomarkers. This will enable a more-comprehensive and holistic understanding of environmental stress triggers and corresponding psychological and biological responses.

References

Russ TC, Stamatakis E, Hamer M, Starr JM, Kivimäki M, Batty GD. Association between psychological distress and mortality: individual participant pooled analysis of 10 prospective cohort studies. BMJ 2012;345 jul31 4:e4933. 10.1136/bmj.e4933 [PMC free article : PMC3409083 ] [PubMed : 22849956 ] [CrossRef]

American Psychological Association, APA Working Group on Stress and Health Disparities. Stress and health disparities: contexts, mechanisms, and interventions among racial/ethnic minority and low socioeconomic status populations. 2017. Available from: https://www ​.apa.org/pi ​/health-equity/resources ​/stress-report.pdf

Selye H. The stress of life. New York (NY): McGraw-Hill; 1956.

Chrousos GP. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat Rev Endocrinol 2009;5(7):374–81. 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106 [PubMed : 19488073 ] [CrossRef]

Koolhaas JM, Bartolomucci A, Buwalda B, de Boer SF, Flügge G, Korte SM … Stress revisited: a critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011;35(5):1291–301. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.003 [PubMed : 21316391 ] [CrossRef]

Weckesser LJ, Dietz F, Schmidt K, Grass J, Kirschbaum C, Miller R. The psychometric properties and temporal dynamics of subjective stress, retrospectively assessed by different informants and questionnaires, and hair cortisol concentrations. Sci Rep 2019;9(1):1098. 10.1038/s41598-018-37526-2 [PMC free article : PMC6355861 ] [PubMed : 30705360 ] [CrossRef]

Monroe SM. Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008;4(1):33–52. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207 [PubMed : 17716038 ] [CrossRef]

Pollak J, Adams P, Gay G. PAM: a photographic affect meter for frequent, in situ measurement of affect. 2011. 10.1145/1978942.1979047 [CrossRef]

Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;54(6):1063–70. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [PubMed : 3397865 ] [CrossRef]

Horowitz M, Wilner N, Alvarez W. Impact of Event Scale: a measure of subjective stress. Psychosom Med 1979;41(3):209–18. 10.1097/00006842-197905000-00004 [PubMed : 472086 ] [CrossRef]

Peacock EJ, Wong PT. The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive appraisal. Stress Med 1990;6(3):227–36. 10.1002/smi.2460060308 [CrossRef]

Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther 1995;33(3):335–43. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U [PubMed : 7726811 ] [CrossRef]

Amirkhan JH. Stress overload: a new approach to the assessment of stress. Am J Community Psychol 2012;49(1-2):55–71. 10.1007/s10464-011-9438-x [PubMed : 21538152 ] [CrossRef]

Kanner AD, Coyne JC, Schaefer C, Lazarus RS. Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. J Behav Med 1981;4(1):1–39. 10.1007/BF00844845 [PubMed : 7288876 ] [CrossRef]

Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983;24(4):385–96. 10.2307/2136404 [PubMed : 6668417 ] [CrossRef]

Gross C, Seebaß K. The Standard Stress Scale (SSS): measuring stress in the life course. In: Blossfeld H-P, von Maurice J, Bayer M, Skopek J, editors. Methodological issues of longitudinal surveys. Springer; 2016. pp. 233–49. 10.1007/978-3-658-11994-2_14 [CrossRef]

Schulz P, Schlotz W, Becker P. Trierer Inventar zum Chronischen Stress. [Trier Inventory for Chronic Stress (TICS)]. TICS; 2004.

Levenstein S, Prantera C, Varvo V, Scribano ML, Berto E, Luzi C … Development of the Perceived Stress Questionnaire: a new tool for psychosomatic research. J Psychosom Res 1993;37(1):19–32. 10.1016/0022-3999(93)90120-5 [PubMed : 8421257 ] [CrossRef]

Gray MJ, Litz BT, Hsu JL, Lombardo TW. Psychometric properties of the life events checklist. Assessment 2004;11(4):330–41. 10.1177/1073191104269954 [PubMed : 15486169 ] [CrossRef]

Brown GW, Harris T. Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. Routledge; 1978.

Petrowski K, Kliem S, Sadler M, Meuret AE, Ritz T, Brähler E. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the English version of the Trier Inventory for Chronic Stress (TICS-E). BMC Med Res Methodol 2018;18(1):18. 10.1186/s12874-018-0471-4 [PMC free article : PMC5801899 ] [PubMed : 29409461 ] [CrossRef]

Remor E. Psychometric properties of a European Spanish version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). Span J Psychol 2006;9(1):86–93. 10.1017/S1138741600006004 [PubMed : 16673626 ] [CrossRef]

Lu W, Bian Q, Wang W, Wu X, Wang Z, Zhao M. Chinese version of the Perceived Stress Scale-10: a psychometric study in Chinese university students. PLoS One 2017;12(12):e0189543. 10.1371/journal.pone.0189543 [PMC free article : PMC5734731 ] [PubMed : 29252989 ] [CrossRef]

Amirkhan JH. A brief stress diagnostic tool: the short stress overload scale. Assessment 2018;25(8):1001–13. 10.1177/1073191116673173 [PubMed : 30392415 ] [CrossRef]

Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008;4(1):1–32. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415 [PubMed : 18509902 ] [CrossRef]

Yang YS, Ryu GW, Choi M. Methodological strategies for ecological momentary assessment to evaluate mood and stress in adult patients using mobile phones: systematic review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth 2019;7(4):e11215. 10.2196/11215 [PMC free article : PMC6462888 ] [PubMed : 30932866 ] [CrossRef]

Jones M, Taylor A, Liao Y, Intille SS, Dunton GF. Real-time subjective assessment of psychological stress: associations with objectively-measured physical activity levels. Psychol Sport Exerc 2017;31:79–87. 10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.03.013 [PMC free article : PMC5685522 ] [PubMed : 29151810 ] [CrossRef]

Razavi T. Self-report measures: an overview of concerns and limitations of questionnaire use in occupational stress research. Discussion Papers in Accounting and Management Science, 01–175. University of Southampton; 2001. Available from: https://eprints ​.soton.ac.uk/35712/

Demetriou C, Özer B, Essau C. Self‐report questionnaires. In The encyclopedia of clinical psychology. Wiley Online Library; 2015. 10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp507 [CrossRef]

Nielsen NR, Kristensen TS, Schnohr P, Grønbaek M. Perceived stress and cause-specific mortality among men and women: results from a prospective cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2008;168(5):481–91. 10.1093/aje/kwn157 [PubMed : 18611955 ] [CrossRef]

Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Bai DS, Lee YH, Koo BH. Stress and heart rate variability: a meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investig 2018;15(3):235–45. 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17 [PMC free article : PMC5900369 ] [PubMed : 29486547 ] [CrossRef]

Charlton PH, Celka P, Farukh B, Chowienczyk P, Alastruey J. Assessing mental stress from the photoplethysmogram: a numerical study. Physiol Meas 2018;39(5):054001. 10.1088/1361-6579/aabe6a [PMC free article : PMC5964362 ] [PubMed : 29658894 ] [CrossRef]

Kilpatrick DG. Differential responsiveness of two electrodermal indices to psychological stress and performance of a complex cognitive task. Psychophysiology 1972;9(2):218–26. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1972.tb00756.x [PubMed : 5024164 ] [CrossRef]

Joyner MJ, Charkoudian N, Wallin BG. Sympathetic nervous system and blood pressure in humans: individualized patterns of regulation and their implications. Hypertension 2010;56(1):10–16. 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.140186 [PMC free article : PMC2891078 ] [PubMed : 20497993 ] [CrossRef]

Devereux RB, Pickering TG, Harshfield GA, Kleinert HD, Denby L, Clark L … Left ventricular hypertrophy in patients with hypertension: importance of blood pressure response to regularly recurring stress. Circulation 1983;68(3):470–6. 10.1161/01.CIR.68.3.470 [PubMed : 6223721 ] [CrossRef]

Dimsdale JE, Moss J. Plasma catecholamines in stress and exercise. JAMA 1980;243(4):340–2. 10.1001/jama.1980.03300300018017 [PubMed : 7351746 ] [CrossRef]

Krantz G, Forsman M, Lundberg U. Consistency in physiological stress responses and electromyographic activity during induced stress exposure in women and men. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 2004;39(2):105–18. 10.1007/BF02734276 [PubMed : 15759598 ] [CrossRef]

Cronstein BN. Interleukin-6—a key mediator of systemic and local symptoms in rheumatoid arthritis. Bull NYU Hosp Jt Dis 2007;65(Suppl 1):S11–5. [PubMed : 17708739 ]

Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Preacher KJ, MacCallum RC, Atkinson C, Malarkey WB, Glaser R. Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100(15):9090–5. 10.1073/pnas.1531903100 [PMC free article : PMC166443 ] [PubMed : 12840146 ] [CrossRef]

Ali N, Nater UM. Salivary alpha-amylase as a biomarker of stress in behavioral medicine. Int J Behav Med 2020;27(3):337–42. 10.1007/s12529-019-09843-x [PMC free article : PMC7250801 ] [PubMed : 31900867 ] [CrossRef]

Aardal E, Holm AC. Cortisol in saliva—reference ranges and relation to cortisol in serum. Eur J Clin Chem Clin Biochem 1995;33(12):927–32. 10.1515/cclm.1995.33.12.927 [PubMed : 8845424 ] [CrossRef]

Remer T, Maser-Gluth C, Wudy SA. Glucocorticoid measurements in health and disease—metabolic implications and the potential of 24-h urine analyses. Mini Rev Med Chem 2008;8(2):153–70. 10.2174/138955708783498096 [PubMed : 18289099 ] [CrossRef]

Baum A, Grunberg N. Measurement of stress hormones. In: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU, editors. Measuring stress: a guide for health and social scientists. Oxford University Press; 1995. pp. 175–92.

Johnson TV, Abbasi A, Master VA. Systematic review of the evidence of a relationship between chronic psychosocial stress and C-reactive protein. Mol Diagn Ther 2013;17(3):147–64. 10.1007/s40291-013-0026-7 [PubMed : 23615944 ] [CrossRef]

Glaser R, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Speicher CE, Holliday JE. Stress, loneliness, and changes in herpesvirus latency. J Behav Med 1985;8(3):249–60. 10.1007/BF00870312 [PubMed : 3003360 ] [CrossRef]

Raul JS, Cirimele V, Ludes B, Kintz P. Detection of physiological concentrations of cortisol and cortisone in human hair. Clin Biochem 2004;37(12):1105–11. 10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2004.02.010 [PubMed : 15589817 ] [CrossRef]

Staufenbiel SM, Penninx BW, Spijker AT, Elzinga BM, van Rossum EF. Hair cortisol, stress exposure, and mental health in humans: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013;38(8):1220–35. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.015 [PubMed : 23253896 ] [CrossRef]

Piazza JR, Almeida DM, Dmitrieva NO, Klein LC. Frontiers in the Use of Biomarkers of Health in Research on Stress and Aging. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2010;65B(5):513–525. 10.1093/geronb/gbq04910.1093/geronb/gbq049 [PMC free article : PMC2920946 ] [PubMed : 20647348 ] [CrossRef] [CrossRef]

Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1994;19(4):313–33. 10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2 [PubMed : 8047637 ] [CrossRef]

Hellhammer DH, Wüst S, Kudielka BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(2):163–71. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026 [PubMed : 19095358 ] [CrossRef]

Wright KD, Hickman R, Laudenslager ML. Hair Cortisol Analysis: A Promising Biomarker of HPA Activation in Older Adults. The Gerontologist. 2015;55 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):S140–S145. 10.1093/geront/gnu17410.1093/geront/gnu174 [PMC free article : PMC4566915 ] [PubMed : 26055775 ] [CrossRef] [CrossRef]

Pragst F, Balikova MA. State of the art in hair analysis for detection of drug and alcohol abuse. Clin Chim Acta 2006;370(1-2):17–49. 10.1016/j.cca.2006.02.019 [PubMed : 16624267 ] [CrossRef]

Godoy LD, Rossignoli MT, Delfino-Pereira P, Garcia-Cairasco N, de Lima Umeoka EH. A comprehensive overview on stress neurobiology: basic concepts and clinical implications. Front Behav Neurosci 2018;12:127. 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00127 [PMC free article : PMC6043787 ] [PubMed : 30034327 ] [CrossRef]

Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull 2004;130(4):601–30. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601 [PMC free article : PMC1361287 ] [PubMed : 15250815 ] [CrossRef]

Glaser R, Pearson GR, Jones JF, Hillhouse J, Kennedy S, Mao HY … Stress-related activation of Epstein-Barr virus. Brain Behav Immun 1991;5(2):219–32. 10.1016/0889-1591(91)90018-6 [PubMed : 1654167 ] [CrossRef]

McDade TW, Stallings JF, Angold A, Costello EJ, Burleson M, Cacioppo JT … Epstein-Barr virus antibodies in whole blood spots: a minimally invasive method for assessing an aspect of cell-mediated immunity. Psychosom Med 2000;62(4):560–8. 10.1097/00006842-200007000-00015 [PubMed : 10949102 ] [CrossRef]

Dowd JB, Aiello AE, Chyu L, Huang YY, McDade TW. Cytomegalovirus antibodies in dried blood spots: a minimally invasive method for assessing stress, immune function, and aging. Immun Ageing 2011;8(1):3. 10.1186/1742-4933-8-3 [PMC free article : PMC3031243 ] [PubMed : 21232134 ] [CrossRef]

Herbert TB, Cohen S. Stress and immunity in humans: a meta-analytic review. Psychosom Med 1993;55(4):364–79. 10.1097/00006842-199307000-00004 [PubMed : 8416086 ] [CrossRef]

Strimbu K, Tavel JA. What are biomarkers? Curr Opin HIV AIDS 2010;5(6):463–6. 10.1097/COH.0b013e32833ed177 [PMC free article : PMC3078627 ] [PubMed : 20978388 ] [CrossRef]

O’Connor MF, Bower JE, Cho HJ, Creswell JD, Dimitrov S, Hamby ME … To assess, to control, to exclude: effects of biobehavioral factors on circulating inflammatory markers. Brain Behav Immun 2009;23(7):887–97. 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.04.005 [PMC free article : PMC2749909 ] [PubMed : 19389469 ] [CrossRef]

Djuric Z, Bird CE, Furumoto-Dawson A, Rauscher GH, Ruffin MT IV, Stowe RP … Biomarkers of psychological stress in health disparities research. Open Biomark J 2008;1:7–19. 10.2174/1875318300801010007 [PMC free article : PMC2841407 ] [PubMed : 20305736 ] [CrossRef]

Schmidt P, Reiss A, Duerichen R, Van Laerhoven K. Wearable affect and stress recognition: a review. [Preprint.] 2018. 10.48550/arXiv.1811.08854 [PMC free article : PMC6806301 ] [PubMed : 31547220 ] [CrossRef]

Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson M, Sollers JJ 3rd, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012;36(2):747–56. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009 [PubMed : 22178086 ] [CrossRef]

Furberg RD, Taniguchi T, Aagaard B, Ortiz AM, Hegarty-Craver M, Gilchrist KH … Biometrics and policing: A protocol for multichannel sensor data collection and exploratory analysis of contextualized psychophysiological response during law enforcement operations. JMIR Res Protoc 2017;6(3):e44. 10.2196/resprot.7499 [PMC free article : PMC5375974 ] [PubMed : 28314707 ] [CrossRef]

Castaneda D, Esparza A, Ghamari M, Soltanpur C, Nazeran H. A review on wearable photoplethysmography sensors and their potential future applications in health care. Int J Biosens Bioelectron 2018;4(4):195–202. [PMC free article : PMC6426305 ] [PubMed : 30906922 ]

Bent B, Goldstein BA, Kibbe WA, Dunn JP. Investigating sources of inaccuracy in wearable optical heart rate sensors. NPJ Digit Med 2020;3(1):18. 10.1038/s41746-020-0226-6 [PMC free article : PMC7010823 ] [PubMed : 32047863 ] [CrossRef]

Drinnan MJ, Allen J, Murray A. Relation between heart rate and pulse transit time during paced respiration. Physiol Meas 2001;22(3):425–32. 10.1088/0967-3334/22/3/301 [PubMed : 11556663 ] [CrossRef]

Perez MV, Mahaffey KW, Hedlin H, Rumsfeld JS, Garcia A, Ferris T …; Apple Heart Study Investigators. Large-scale assessment of a smartwatch to identify atrial fibrillation. N Engl J Med 2019;381(20):1909–17. 10.1056/NEJMoa1901183 [PMC free article : PMC8112605 ] [PubMed : 31722151 ] [CrossRef]

Garbarino M, Lai M, Bender D, Picard RW, Tognetti S. Empatica E3 — a wearable wireless multi-sensor device for real-time computerized biofeedback and data acquisition. In: 2014 4th International Conference on Wireless Mobile Communication and Healthcare - Transforming Healthcare Through Innovations in Mobile and Wireless Technologies. MOBIHEALTH; 2014:39–42.

Liu Y, Du S. Psychological stress level detection based on electrodermal activity. Behav Brain Res 2018;341:50–3. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.021 [PubMed : 29274343 ] [CrossRef]

Reinhardt T, Schmahl C, Wüst S, Bohus M. Salivary cortisol, heart rate, electrodermal activity and subjective stress responses to the Mannheim Multicomponent Stress Test (MMST). Psychiatry Res 2012;198(1):106–11. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.009 [PubMed : 22397919 ] [CrossRef]

Walvekar SS, Ambekar JG, Devaranavadagi BB. Study on serum cortisol and perceived stress scale in the police constables. J Clin Diagn Res 2015;9(2):BC10–4. 10.7860/JCDR/2015/12015.5576 [PMC free article : PMC4378726 ] [PubMed : 25859444 ] [CrossRef]

Amirkhan JH, Urizar GG, Clark S. Criterion validation of a stress measure: the Stress Overload Scale. Psychol Assess 2015;27(3):985–96. 10.1037/pas0000081 [PMC free article : PMC6309273 ] [PubMed : 25642927 ] [CrossRef]

O’Brien KM, Tronick EZ, Moore CL. Relationship between hair cortisol and perceived chronic stress in a diverse sample. Stress Health 2013;29(4):337–44. 10.1002/smi.2475 [PubMed : 23225553 ] [CrossRef]

Lehrer HM, Steinhardt MA, Dubois SK, Laudenslager ML. Perceived stress, psychological resilience, hair cortisol concentration, and metabolic syndrome severity: a moderated mediation model. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2020;113:104510. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104510 [PMC free article : PMC7769194 ] [PubMed : 31911349 ] [CrossRef]

Achsah Dorsey, PhD, is an assistant professor of Anthropology at the University of Massachusetts—Amherst.

Elissa Scherer is a public health analyst at RTI International.

Randy Eckhoff is a research programmer at RTI International.

Robert Furberg, PhD MBA, is a clinical informaticist.

RTI International is an independent, nonprofit research organization dedicated to improving the human condition. The RTI Press mission is to disseminate information about RTI research, analytic tools, and technical expertise to a national and international audience. RTI Press publications are peer-reviewed by at least two independent substantive experts and one or more Press editors.

Suggested Citation

Dorsey, A., Scherer, E., Eckhoff, R., and Furberg, R. (2022). Measurement of Human Stress: A Multidimensional Approach. RTI Press Publication No. OP-0073-2206. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI Press. https://doi.org/10.3768/rtipress.2022.op.0073.2206

RTI International is an independent, nonprofit research institute dedicated to improving the human condition. We combine scientific rigor and technical expertise in social and laboratory sciences, engineering, and international development to deliver solutions to the critical needs of clients worldwide.